Monday, September 21, 2009

WWJD? (What Would [Sam] Johnson Do?): Legit Words From "Spectacular" to "Craptacular"



Happy Bday, Sam J.

It's pretty spectacular to get media attention for your 300th birthday. Samuel Johnson, who famously defined "lexicographer," in part, as a "harmless drudge," seemingly putting down his own major life's work, was still a (minor) media celebrity on his tercentenary this month-- September 18.

From other definitions in Johnson's path-breaking 1755 dictionary, it's clear he was not overly burdened with modesty. But he probably would not have foreseen such long-lasting recognition. He certainly could not have foreseen the spectacular twists and turns roiling dictionary-making today. How, one wonders, would he react to the radically new options?

National Public Radio, for example, celebrated Johnson's birthday with a lively story on historical changes in the standards for including and defining words, set by the leading English dictionaries on both sides of "the pond." (www.onthemedia.org/transcripts/2009/09/18/06) [The story is called, I might possessively note: "Defining Moments"]

Talking Out of School- But the reporter, Mike Vuolo, focused on what now, almost 50 years later, seems to be a quaint furor -- the 1961 Third Edition of Merriam-Webster's "New International Dictionary." What caused widespread indignation at the time? M-W's Third included slang, curse words and, in general, words, spellings, and definitions that were in common use, instead of limiting the contents to the "standard" (some might say, "elitist") form of language as taught in schools.

Letting Common Users In? Reporter Vuolo saw the irony in that edition's now being viewed as a hoary authority. And he began to pry open the door on the aspect of 21st century dictionary-making that our hero Sam Johnson would find most bewildering -- the many ways that "common" users are getting involved in the dictionary-making process.

Johnson had some help in his innovation of documenting many entries with literary quotations, but basically his dictionary was a massive one-man operation. Having users supply new words or create definitions would have seemed beyond the pale.

NPR's Vuolo interviewed editor-lexicographers of two avant-garde entrants in the dictionary field which do reach beyond the pale and accept user-suggested entries:
Vuolo flung a zinger of a word --"craptacular" -- at the editor-lexicographers to see how inclusive their user-aided products really were. He had first encountered the term last year, in a Wall Street Journal reporter's description of the emerging financial free-fall. Reputable source, but was that really a word? In Wordnik? Yes, for sure. Open Dictionary? Yes, again. Merriam-Webster's Third, online edition? No way.

User-involvement vs. User-control. We'll be following Wordnik and Open Dictionary from a special perspective: Their role in the changing social structure of the practices, and the profession, involved in formal defining. For now, the point is that both those innovating dictionaries still are controlled by very young versions of Sam Johnson, i.e., lexicographers.

Maybe because it would have been ungracious on Johnson's B-day, NPR didn't even mention the truly avant avant-garde in dictionaries -- those that are user-controlled.
Of course, "craptacular" appears in both -- in fact, with 50 definitions in Urban, some of them being, well, quite craptacular.

And, Yes, "Bday" is a Dictionary Word --at least it is in user-controlled Wiktionary and Urban, but not yet in "harmless drudge-controlled" Open Dictionary and Wordnik.

Monday, September 7, 2009

Strange [Definitions of] Behavior

A Different Take on “Defining Experiences”

“Defining experiences” can be ordinary. The usual take on that term is “life-changing events”, but I’m referring to the unnoticed occasions in a typical day when we weave definitions into conversations. Those informal definitions are like grease that keeps social gears shifting smoothly.

Sure, lots of social behavior happens smoothly without defining either the terms of the relationship, or the terms that the behavior is about. You and I can go shopping without defining what shopping is, and without defining whether we are BFF (Best Friends Forever) or just acquaintances who enjoy an outing to Target. (But if you show up seeking Saks and I was targeting Target, we may need to stop and define what shopping means.)

Science versus “Daily Life”

However, defining is crucially important in some specialized arenas, especially the Sciences -- or so we’ve come to believe. Science progresses because researchers can build on each others’ findings, and to do that they must have agreed-upon technical definitions of the key things they are measuring, right? Not necessarily.

Man Bites Dog? Earlier this summer—July 21, 2009 to be precise -- the New York Times ran what amounts to a Science section version of the model mock news story: “Man Bites Dog!” Reporter Natalie Angier got the scoop on the shocking revelation that Behavioral Biologists do not agree on a definition of a truly central term in their specialty: the word is “behavior.” Angier drew her facts from a study in the July 2009 issue of the journal Animal Behaviour, whose main conclusions were:

biologists don't agree with one another on what a behavior is;”

Indeed, the study compared biologists’ responses in two parts of the surveys they filled out, and found that:

“biologists don't agree with themselves on what a behavior is”! {italics added)

This definitional disarray in a science specialty seems more serious than common differences in daily-life definitions. Who got the idea in the first place to do this bit of investigative research? Was it a renowned scholar secure enough in his or her career to raise this professionally sensitive issue? Surprisingly, no.

The Emperor Has No Clothes? The story fulfilled another model for media interest, since if you recall, it was a naïve and therefore painfully honest little boy who proclaimed the Emperor’s nakedness. Angier saw the irony in noting who sparked the study that revealed the definitional hairy situation that all those Behavioral Biologists were parading around in.

It was a graduate student, Daniel Levitis, who innocently asked his professor for a good technical definition of “behavior.” Finding a mish-mash of definitions when he searched textbooks and glossaries of research reports, Levitis went on to conduct a survey of about 175 researchers, and to be the lead author of the journal article (Levitis DA, Lidicker, Jr WZ, & Freund G, “Behavioural biologists do not agree on what constitutes behaviour.)”

Defining terms: For What, By Whom?

So, we are left wondering: Are shared definitions really needed for cumulative research? Might definitions serve other, more social, functions even in academic Science? Put better, under what conditions are definitions really needed?

Or are definitions in science, as in daily life, at least in part a way that some people pull symbolic power plays over others in attributing meanings, and having their definitions gain wider acceptance? Hot-button questions like those argue for looking more closely at “defining experiences.”

%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%%